What can we do to reduce the amount of research that goes unpublished?

An excellent piece was published by Paul Glasziou and Iain Chalmers about the large percentage of research that goes unpublished. As they note, the estimate that 50% goes unpublished is likely an underestimate. Unfortunately they didn’t offer any significant solutions other than we need a “better understanding of the causes of, and cures for, non-publication.”

A “simple” solution is for the drug/device approval process to require all studies related to that product and/or conducted/funded by the requesting company be registered and published. This would miss studies done after drug/device approval or done by independent parties but a large number of nonpublished studies are conducted or funded by the companies that market the drug/device. This would also miss all the other studies not directly related to drug/devices (e.g. epidemiological studies).

Another significant challenge is where to publish this information. The web makes the most sense as this is the cheapest route of publication. Maybe the FDA (or some international commission) could have a page(s) on each drug that includes full text access to all studies done on that drug/device. Would these need peer and editorial review? Yes, but a daunting task as we already struggle to find willing and competent peer reviewers. FDA budgets shrink repeatedly and this would be a significant financial burden.

What I really wanted to do in this post was to give my thoughts on a  question raised by Jon Brassey (Director of the TRIP Database):

  • What is better a large RCT or a SR based on a “biased subsample”?

Is a large RCT more desirable than a systematic review (SR) based on a biased subsample of studies? This has been a conundrum for some time. You can argue both sides of this. The reason he says biased subsample is that we know more positive studies get published than negative, larger effects get published more than small effects, etc. Is the answer to this question “it depends”? It depends on your goals: a more precise estimate of the biased effect (favors SR), more generalizability (favors SR), a potentially more methodologically sound result (favors RCT). What is interesting to consider is that the same study repeated over and over will result in a distribution of results (this is why it shouldn’t surprise us that when we do seemingly the same study we don’t get the exact same result). Should we repeat studies? When should we stop repeating the studies (i.e. when have we adequately defined the distribution of results)?

I don’t think we can really answer this question as both of these study types have limitations but if I had to pick one I would rather have a large RCT that is well done than a SR based on a limited subset of the data especially considering we don’t know what is missing and the effect seen in those missing studies.

 

What to do when evidence has validity issues?

I often wonder how different clinicians (and EBM gurus) approach the dilemma of critically appraising  an article only to find that it has a flaw(s). For example, a common flaw is lack of concealed allocation in a randomized controlled trial. Empirical studies show that the effects of experimental interventions are exaggerated by about 21% [ratio of odds ratios (ROR): 0.79, 95% CI: 0.66–0.95] when allocation concealment is unclear or inadequate (JAMA 1995;273:40812). 

bias1

So what should I do if the randomized trial doesn’t adequately conceal the allocation scheme? I could discard the study completely and look for another study. What if there isn’t another study? Should I ignore the data of a perfectly good study otherwise? I could use the study and adjust the findings down by 21% (see above for why) and if the effect of the intervention  still crosses my clinically important threshold then I would implement the therapy. I could use the study as is and assume it wasn’t important because the reviewers and editors didn’t think it was. This is foolish as many of them probably didn’t even recognize the flaw nor would many of them understand the impact.

I don’t have the right answer but wonder what more learned people do. I personally adjust the findings down and determine if I still want to use the information. The problem with this approach is it assumes that in the particular study I am reviewing that the estimate of effect is in fact biased…something I can’t really know.

What do you do?

Podcasting to help keep current

Intro image

I am taking a class on Multimedia as part of my Master of Educational Technology degree program. This week our assignment was to develop a podcast and I decided to make it EBM related (always make your work count twice). I used Audacity, a free audio editor and recorder, to create the podcast. There was a learning curve but I have it mostly figured out. In the past when I created all my YouTube videos I “lectured” off the top of my head. For this assignment I had to write a script first and read from it. This is much better than ad-libbing. I don’t have an verbal tics (like “uhs”) and my cadence is better.  I suggest if you do any recordings, even about things you know a lot about, make a script and read it.

Medicine Review in a Few will be a podcast series in which I review what I consider important studies in Internal Medicine.  Each episode will review one study and will last less than 10 minutes; hence the “in a few” portion of the title. I think its important to keep information that isn’t interactive and is only processed through one channel fairly short. I personally lose interest and focus with long podcasts. According to data from Stitcher.com the average listener abandons a podcast within 22 minutes.

In Episode 1 I review the ADJUST-PE study. I chose to begin my podcast series with this study because I recently used the information in this study to care for a patient. I wasn’t aware of the findings of this study until one of my residents brought it to my attention.  I plan to only review clinically useful studies and will comment on any methodological limitations of the studies that I think the average clinician wouldn’t recognize or know how that limitation impacts the study findings. I think podcasts are a good medium to review studies.

For now, the podcasts will only be posted here but if I keep up with this endeaver I’ll ultimately try to get them on iTunes.

The image I used above is from splitshire.com and requires no attribution. The music used in my podcast is royalty free from Looperman.com.

2014 in review

The WordPress.com stats helper monkeys prepared a 2014 annual report for this blog.

Here’s an excerpt:

The concert hall at the Sydney Opera House holds 2,700 people. This blog was viewed about 8,400 times in 2014. If it were a concert at Sydney Opera House, it would take about 3 sold-out performances for that many people to see it.

Click here to see the complete report.

Allocation Concealment Is Often Confused With Blinding

During journal clubs on randomized controlled trials there is often confusion about allocation concealment. It is often confused with blinding. In a sense it is blinding but not in the traditional sense of blinding. One way to think of allocation concealment is blinding of the randomization schedule or scheme. Allocation concealment hides the randomization or allocation sequence (what’s coming next) from patients and those who would enroll patients in a study. Blinding occurs after randomization and keeps patients, providers, researchers, etc from knowing which arm of the study the patient is in (i.e. what treatment they are getting).

Why is allocation concealment important in a randomized controlled trial? Inadequate or unclear allocation concealment can lead to an overestimation (by up to 40%!) of treatment effect (JAMA 1995;273:408). First, consider why we randomize in the first place. We randomize to try to equally distribute confounding and prognostic factors between arms of a study so we can try to isolate the effect of the intervention. Consider a physician who wants to enroll a patient in a study and wants to make sure her patient receives the therapy she deems likely most effective. What if she figured out the randomization scheme and knows what therapy the next patient will be assigned to? Hopefully you can see that this physician could undermine the benefits of randomization if she preferentially funnels sicker (or healthier) patients into one arm of the study. There could be an imbalance in baseline characteristics. It could also lead to patients who are enrolled in the study being fundamentally different or not representative of the patient population.

From The Lancet

From The Lancet

You will have to use your judgment to decide how likely it is that someone could figure out the randomization scheme. You can feel more comfortable that allocation concealment was adequate if the following were used in the RCT:
sequentially numbered, opaque, sealed envelopes: these are not able to be seen through even if held up to a light. They are sealed so that you can’t peek into them and see what the assignment is. As each patient is enrolled you use the next numbered envelope.
pharmacy controlled: enrolling physician calls the pharmacy and they enroll the patient and assign therapy.
centralized randomization: probably the most commonly used. The enrolling physician calls a central research site and the central site assigns the patient to therapy.

Proper randomization is crucial to a therapy study and concealed allocation is crucial to randomization. I hope this post helps readers of RCTs better understand what concealed allocation is and learn how to detect whether it was done adequately or not. Keep in mind if allocation concealment is unclear or done poorly the effect you see in the study needs to be tempered and possible cut by 40%.